Page 1 of 18

Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal – Vol. 9, No. 9

Publication Date: September 25, 2022

DOI:10.14738/assrj.99.13030. Zheng, H., Elliott, W., & O’Brien, M. (2022). The Importance of Parent/Child Communications About Children’s Savings Accounts for

Developing a College-Bound Identity. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 9(9). 173-190.

Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom

The Importance of Parent/Child Communications About

Children’s Savings Accounts for Developing a College-Bound

Identity

Haotian Zheng

University of Michigan School of Social Work

1080 S University Ave, Ann Arbor, MI 48109 USA

William Elliott

University of Michigan School of Social Work

1080 S University Ave, Ann Arbor, MI 48109 USA

Megan O’Brien

University of Michigan School of Social Work

1080 S University Ave, Ann Arbor, MI 48109 USA

ABSTRACT

Children’s Savings Accounts (CSAs) not only can help children and families to pay

for expenses related to postsecondary education but also produce positive social

and psychological effects, such as children’s college-bound identity. In this sense,

CSAs provide a rich context for parent and child to communicate about college and

ultimately for children to co-construct a college-bound identity with parents. This

study examines the relationship between parent/child communication about

participation in CSA and children’s college-bound identity. Using data from 506

parent/child dyads participating in the Promise Scholars CSA, we examine if

parent/child communications about their CSA are associated with a stronger

college-bound identity, an important predictor of future academic success.

Propensity score weighting is performed to minimize the impact of selection bias.

Multiple imputations are performed to handle missing data. We find evidence of

higher college-bound identity scores among communicator dyads compared to non- communicator dyads and the comparison group. We also find that the difference in

college-bound identity scores is not statistically significant between non- communicator dyads and the comparison group. Given this finding, CSA programs

may consider interventions that may facilitate parent/child communications about

their CSA as a way of encouraging children to form a college-bound identity. Further,

we posit that parent/child communications about CSA are another important way

for programs to understand engagement as most programs aim to increase college

attendance. Until recently, CSA programs have often narrowly understood

engagement as whether families make contributions or not.

Key words: Promise Scholars, Children’s Savings Accounts, Assets, College-Bound

Identity, Parent/Child Communication

Page 2 of 18

174

Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ) Vol. 9, Issue 9, September-2022

Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom

Highlights:

• Students of the communicator group have stronger college-bound identity than students

of the comparison group as well as the non-communicator group.

• Students of the non-communicator group did not have statistically different college- bound identity scores than children in the comparison group.

Highlights:

• The college-bound identity of communicator group student is higher than the non- communicator group and the comparison group.

• The college-bound identity of students in the non-communicator group is not different

than children in the comparison group.

INTRODUCTION

In their simplest form, Children’s Savings Accounts (CSAs), sometimes called Child

Development Accounts (CDAs) are asset-building accounts for children. In their current state,

these accounts usually have the goal of helping children pay for expenses related to

postsecondary education, but they can be used for broader asset building activities (e.g., saving

for a home, retirement, or a business). In addition to providing families with an account for

making their own contributions, they augment families’ efforts by providing them with an

initial deposit, incentives, and matching funds. According to Prosperity Now [1], by the end of

2020, there were 109 active CSA programs serving about 922,000 children in more than 36

states. This was a 30% increase from the end of 2019. Typically, CSA programs start at birth or

when children enter kindergarten. While family contributions are important within CSA

programs, it is not the only, or even main mechanism that CSAs provide for building assets.

When a child has an account, the scaffolding is put in place to augment the saving efforts of low- income families through government, philanthropy, and other sources. Some programs are

experimenting with targeted ongoing deposits as a means of building assets, some universities

are matching family savings up to a specified amount, while some foundations are converting

traditional scholarships into early award scholarships.

By augmenting low-income families’ contributions with initial deposits, matches, incentives, as

well as the opportunity for third party contributions, CSAs mimic early educational transfers

wealthy families provide to their children. These early investments in a child’s educational

trajectory are crucial for their outcomes as adults. Research shows that predicted household

income and net worth are higher for adults who received parental financial support for college

as children than for those receiving no such support [2]. Because CSAs are often designed

specifically with the goal in mind of providing low-income children financial support for college,

we posit, like early educational transfers of wealthy families, CSAs have the potential for

increasing income and wealth in adulthood among children born into low-income households.

When families make contributions to their child’s CSA, they are engaging with its wealth

building capacity. However, as Sherraden [3] notes, CSAs also can create social and

psychological effects. When Sherraden [3] discusses CSAs’ ability to create social and

psychological effects, he primarily talks about this from an institutional perspective; how

institutions shape individuals and society. But like with building assets, the institution not only

provides the structure needed for asset accumulation (e.g., initial deposits, matches, the

account itself), it also provides the opportunity for families to engage in building assets through

Page 3 of 18

175

Zheng, H., Elliott, W., & O’Brien, M. (2022). The Importance of Parent/Child Communications About Children’s Savings Accounts for Developing a

College-Bound Identity. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 9(9). 173-190.

URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.99.13030

their own individual contributions. This is what most human behavior looks like, institutions

augment what people can do (or provide the bounds on what they can do). At the same time,

you cannot fully understand the impact of institutions on behavior without accounting for

differing levels of effort and ability by individuals. And so, in as much as CSAs create social and

psychological effects, it also means at the same time they provide individuals with the

opportunity to engage in producing social and psychological effects above what the institution

can create on its own.

Acknowledging that both institutions and individuals play a key role in determining individual

outcomes, in this study, we investigate how might families engage with CSAs’ capacity as an

institution for producing social and psychological effects? We suggest one way is through

parent/child communications. From this perspective, CSAs not only facilitate greater asset

accumulation among low-income families, but they also facilitate communication between

parent and child about college and the need to begin planning for it now, not later. We use

identity-based motivation theory developed and tested by Daphna Oyserman and her

colleagues to make specific hypotheses [4-6].

While it is not the focus of this paper, we think it is still important to recognize individual

outcomes are not solely determined by institutions or individuals’ effort, and ability; history

also plays a role. In America, there is a long history of inequitable wealth distribution [7]. This

history impacts the return low-income and Black children can receive from education alone.

For example, research shows low-income children as adults with a four-year degree earn about

one-third less than higher-income families with a four-year degree [8]. Regarding wealth,

research shows Black families who have a head of household who graduated from college have

about 33% less wealth than White families who have a head of household who dropped out of

high school [9]. And so, if the goal is to ultimately reduce wealth inequality, access to small- dollar CSAs (initial deposits of $5 to $1,000) will not be enough, nor will individuals own use of

effort and ability. Substantial targeted ongoing deposits are needed to overcome existing

wealth gaps that derive from a legacy of wealth inequality in America.

REVIEW OF RESEARCH

Research on CSAs has for some time theorized that it matters if children know whether they

have an account or not (e.g., [10]). Early on this was articulated as children having an account

in their own name with the assumption being, if they have an account but are not aware of it, it

will not produce the kinds of social and psychological effects that have facilitated the growth of

CSAs. The focus on whether the account was in the child’s name was driven by available data at

the time. Early studies, before primary data was available, largely relied on secondary data from

the Panel Study of Income Dynamics (PSID). The Child Development Supplement (CDS) of the

PSID asks children first if they have a bank account in their name and then whether they have

money set aside in that account for future schooling such as college. What became clearer as

program data became available, was not so much that the account is in the child’s name, but

that the child has knowledge that money was being put aside for them in an account to attend

college.

This paper builds on these nascent ideas within the CSA field, that children who know that

money is being put aside for them in college savings accounts are more likely to form an identity

as being college-bound than those who are not aware, despite being in a CSA program. One way

Page 4 of 18

176

Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ) Vol. 9, Issue 9, September-2022

Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom

to measure that knowledge is by asking parents if they are communicating with their children

about participating in the CSA program. Why communication is important is because programs

can have a direct impact on parental communications. More specifically, if parent/child

communications are found to be an important predictor of a child’s formation of a college- bound identity, interventions can be easily developed that help remind parents of the

importance of talking to their children about their CSA.

Educational Expectations, used as a Proxy for College-Bound Identity

There is a well-established body of research in the education field that indicates parental and

children’s college expectations are strong predictors of children’s educational success (e.g., [10-

12]). Moreover, research shows CSAs are associated with children having more positive

expectations about their ability to attend college (e.g.,[10]). Drawing on research done by social

psychologist Daphne Oyserman and her team (e.g.,[4-6]), researchers have suggested that CSAs

help children form and act on their college-bound identity [13-15]. Children’s college

expectations have been used by researchers as a proxy for college-bound identity (e.g., [13]).

The idea that children form a college-bound identity is based on Identity-Based Motivation

theory (IBM) which has three key components: salience (images of future selves are on the

mind now), group congruence (people like me also go to college), and difficulty as normal [16].

These core components have been shown to be important predictors of children’s school

behaviors [16] and may help researchers understand more about how CSAs influence children’s

behaviors (e.g., [14-15]). In a recent paper, Elliott et al. [14] use a preliminary instrument to

measure children’s college-bound identity. They find that participating in a CSA program is

associated with children (fourth through ninth grades) forming a college-bound identity.

Parental Educational Communication

Building on the early thinking about CSA awareness and resulting influence on the way children

think about their future, this paper investigates the role of parental communications within CSA

programs. Parental communications generally fall under the study of parenting practices.

Parenting practices have been shown to be an important factor for understanding child

functioning [17]. According to Darling and Steinberg [18], parental practice consists of parental

attitudes and patterned behaviors (such as parental involvement to include communication,

structure, discipline, psychological control, and other behaviors) which make up the parent- child relationship. While parenting practices are influenced by several different domains, the

domain most relevant to the relationship between CSAs and parenting practices is family

context [19]. A part of the family context domain is socioeconomic status (e.g., income, wealth,

employment) (for a complete list of domains see [20]).

In a statewide randomized control trial conducted by the Center for Social Development (CSD)

at Washington University, Huang et al. [21] found that CSAs are causally related to a reduction

in punitive-parenting practices, even a tool for positive parent-child interactions. Researchers

from CSD also found that CSAs reduce maternal depression [22] and result in more positive

parental educational expectations for their children [23]. Qualitative research suggests that

parents in CSA programs may be more likely to talk to their children about college [24].

However, research on CSAs has not examined whether these communications may increase the

likelihood that children develop a positive college-bound identity. Parental communications

might be a way that CSAs influence children’s development of a college-bound identity, and

Page 5 of 18

177

Zheng, H., Elliott, W., & O’Brien, M. (2022). The Importance of Parent/Child Communications About Children’s Savings Accounts for Developing a

College-Bound Identity. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 9(9). 173-190.

URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.99.13030

importantly, point to a way for programs to strengthen CSA effects through interventions that

encourage families to communicate with their children about their CSAs.

THEORY

In this paper we posit that parental communication about CSAs is associated with children

forming a college-bound identity. We use Identity-Based Motivation (IBM) theory to

understand how parent-child communications about CSAs is related to children’s formation of

a college-bound identity. IBM is a social psychological theory that explains how people form

identities (possible identity or possible self) and under which situations people are likely to act

on an identity [6, 25-26]. A possible identity is a child’s perception of their potential to become

a certain type of person in the future within a particular social and cultural context [27-29].

From this perspective, a college-bound identity is a child’s perception of their potential for

becoming a college student in the future within their particular social and cultural context. A

college-bound identity would come with a set of personal traits and characteristics about what

it means to be college-bound [30]. People form many possible [16] that are positive or negative

[30]. People do not act on each identity they form (for a review, [31]). To be actionable, possible

identities must be linked to strategies to reach them and important for the current context [32].

However, this study is focused on the formation of identities not on whether children act on

them.

Drawing from IBM, we hypothesize a way college-bound identity develops is through parent- child conversations regarding college and career within the context of being a CSA participant.

CSAs provide an opportunity for families to engage in conversations with their children about

college. For example, most CSAs have as one of their primary goals, children being able to pay

for college. They are often administered through a state 529 plan. State 529 plans are tax- advantaged savings plans designed specifically for the purpose of helping families pay for

college. As such, CSAs by their very nature provide families with a context for talking about

college with their children. It is not only the structure of CSAs that provides the context for

families to talk to their children about college, but it is also the program materials and quarterly

financial statements that families receive. For example, in a qualitative study of SEED OK

participants, researchers find, “Many respondents say that the SEED OK program materials and

quarterly SEED OK 529 statements remind them that their child has an account for

postsecondary education and prompt them to think about saving.” So, these material and

statements cue to parents that their child has a strategy for going to college, bringing it to the

forefront of their minds and providing them with another opportunity to talk to their children

about their account and what it is for. In this way, CSAs provide parents and children the

opportunity to talk about their CSA and going to college.

Importantly, evidence suggests parent-child communicationns are key to children’s formation

of possible-identities [33-35, 28]. In essence, the parent and child co-construct the child’s

college bound identity [35]. Marshall et al.’s [35] findings that parent and child co-construct the

child’s possible identities is supported by research that finds parent and child often share

similar possible identities for the child [36]. Therefore, we posit parent and child co-construct

the child’s college-bound identity. Further, we posit that CSAs provide a rich context for parent

and child to communicate about college and ultimately for children to co-construct their

college-bound identity with their parent. It is important to note, however, the child does not

blindly accept parental views about what their college-bound identity should be. Instead,

Page 6 of 18

178

Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ) Vol. 9, Issue 9, September-2022

Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom

research shows that children bring into the conversation information from other contexts

which allows them to alter parent views to fit their own views [37]. And so, individuals and

their experience also matter and will help explain whether children form a college-bound

identity.

Promise Scholars

In this paper, we use data from Promise Scholars. Promise Scholars is a CSA program started

by the Community Foundation of Wabash County (CFWC) for children in Wabash County,

Indiana. The program serves students in grades 4-8 by linking a CSA with additional

opportunities to regularly earn scholarships and match by satisfying objective criteria. Earned

scholarship awards captured in their CSA are later granted (up to the student’s age of 23) for

qualified educational expenses once a student enrolls in a qualifying post-secondary institution.

For a full description of the Promise Scholars program and the activities and amounts of the

scholarships provided see [14].

METHODS

The population of this study included a total of 1,579 fifth, sixth, seventh, and ninth grade

students attending Wabash County schools during the 2019-2020 academic year. The

population was made up of 413 fifth graders, 393 sixth graders, 404 seventh graders, and 369

ninth graders. Surveys were separately conducted for parents and students. The Promise

Scholars program has partnered with the University of Southern California and Summitlab

Corporation to conduct a randomized controlled trial (RCT) of Pathways to Success. Pathways is

a 12-session classroom-based social-psychological intervention that aims to help eighth grade

students transition to high school. It is implemented twice a week during the first six weeks of

the school year. Therefore, the current study did not include eighth grade students in the study

population. We plan to interview eighth grade students for future studies.

Data

Parent Survey

Parents of 1,579 fifth, sixth, seventh, and ninth grade students were distributed surveys

through email, with follow-up reminders via mail and at school-based events. CFWC provided

the mailing list, to which links to an online survey were mailed with periodic reminders before

the closure of the survey. Eighty-seven percent of the parents had access to emails and received

online invitations to complete the survey. The response rate to the online survey was 44%.

Along with the parent survey, a comprehensive consent form was presented to explain, and

seek consent for, the child survey. For families that had multiple children in the eligible sample,

a separate email with a unique survey link was sent for each child. Respondents received a $20

pre-paid debit card for each survey completed.

There were about 100 items in the parent survey, which covered a broad range of topics

including: parent engagement in students’ academic activities, knowledge of savings programs

and communication with child about savings early childhood education and childcare, and

discussion of post-secondary goals; parental educational expectations, planning/saving for

post-secondary education; parent and child health history, academic performance, material

hardships; select demographics about primary and secondary caregiver including

race/ethnicity, education, employment, household income, and net worth.

Page 7 of 18

179

Zheng, H., Elliott, W., & O’Brien, M. (2022). The Importance of Parent/Child Communications About Children’s Savings Accounts for Developing a

College-Bound Identity. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 9(9). 173-190.

URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.99.13030

Student Survey

Each school provided a place for students whose parents consented to participate in the child

study to complete the survey. Information about the student survey was emailed to eligible

students one-day before the survey date. In groups by grade, a CFWC or school representative

read the consent information out and began the survey. To indicate completion of the survey,

the student showed the facilitator the final screen that indicated the survey has been completed.

Each student received $10 cash for taking part in the survey. CFWC followed up with students

absent on the date of the survey to ensure they had the opportunity to participate in the study.

Child Assessment Tool- Electronic (CHAT-E), developed by Terri Sabol and her team at

Northwestern University, was used to collect child data. Preliminary findings from

kindergartners indicated that CHAT-E had adequate construct validity [38]. CHAT-E consisted

of 36 statement items and two open-ended questions on children’s perceptions of college,

school, and experience with money and savings. By design, CHAT-E was a developmentally

informed tool that assessed how young children made meaning of school and college. CHAT-E

for young children was administered on a tablet application. Informed by the Berkeley puppet

interview, CHAT-E contained puppet-like exchange on a tablet showing two identical animated

figures who made different statements on same topics such as school, college, and money (To

learn more about the Berkeley puppet interview see

https://dslab.uoregon.edu/about/berkeley-puppet-interview/). Students were prompted to

select puppets whose statement they mostly agreed with.

Given the population with older students for the current study, we collaborated with CHAT-E

developers to modify animated items for online and paper-and-pencil modalities. Still, as with

the original, each student was presented with a pair of statements and asked to select the one

that was most like them. Despite the age differences, all items except for one, retained the

original wording. One item about parents buy toys was changed to “nice things”. Among 637

complete parent surveys, 93% (n = 594) consented for child to take part in the student survey.

Sample

There were 506 parents and students in the final analytic sample. Forty-six students were not

included in the sample because they had an Indiana CollegeChoice 529 Direct Savings Plan but

were not enrolled in the Community Foundation Promise Scholarship program. Seventy-eight

complete student surveys without a matched parent survey were removed. Therefore, the

analytic sample for children’s college-bound identity consist of 506 matched parent and student

pairs.

In the analytic sample of the current study, half of the students were female, 24% were fifth

graders, 27 % were sixth graders, 28 % were seventh graders, and 21 % were ninth graders.

The composition of surveyed students from different grades was comparable to the

composition of the population. Among parents, 34% received a bachelor’s degree or above,

41% earned less than $55,000 in 2019, 73% were married, and 95% were white. According to

Census Bureau [39], 18% of Wabash County residents received a bachelor’s degree or above,

the median household income was $50,637 in 2018, 52% residents were married, and 97%

residents identified as white. Therefore, the parent sample were made up of more college- educated, married, and higher-income respondents than typical residents of Wabash County.

Page 8 of 18

180

Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ) Vol. 9, Issue 9, September-2022

Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom

Measures

The variable of interest in this study was a three-level variable constructed based on students’

Promise Scholar status (1 = Promise Scholars; 0 = Comparison) and communication between

parents and students about Promise Scholars (1 = Communicated; 0 = No communication).

Regarding communications, respondents were specifically asked, “In the past 12 months, did

you or another adult in your household talk with [child] specifically about [child]’s Promise 529

account or the Community Foundation Promise Early Award Scholarship Fund?” Students in the

comparison group (not in Promise Scholars) were coded 0, parents of Promise Scholars who

did not communicate about the program were coded 1, and parents of Promise Scholars who

communicated about the program was coded 2.

Dependent variables

College-bound identity was measured with three subscales (i.e., salience, difficulty as normal,

and group congruence). The three subscales consist of 27 items altogether: identity salience

with 7 items, group congruence with 10 items, and interpretation of difficulty as normal with

10 items. Five items were removed after unidimensionality and reliability tests. The total score

of college-bound identity ranges from 0 to 22, with a higher score indicating a more positive

college-bound identity. The composite reliability and the Alpha of the scale were respectively

0.72 and 0.67 [14], which indicate acceptable reliability.

Control variables

There were seven control variables used in this study including child’s gender (1 = Female; 0 =

Male), child’s overall academic performance perceived by one parent (1 = Above average; 0 =

Average or worse), child’s health status rated by one parent (1 = Excellent; 0 = Very good or

worse), child’s grade (1 = fifth grade, 2 = sixth grade, 3 = seventh grade, and 4 = ninth grade),

parent marital status (1 = Married; 0 = Not married), household net worth, and household

income.

Analytic strategy

The following hypotheses were tested in this study:

H1: Promise Scholars whose parents communicated about the program with them (i.e.

communicators) have higher college-bound identity than do the comparison group.

H2: Promise Scholars whose parents communicate about the program with them (i.e.

communicators) have higher college-bound identity than do Promise Scholars whose parents

do not communicate about the program with them (i.e. non-communicators).

Three steps of analysis were conducted in this study. The first step was to analyze the pattern

of missing data to determine whether the default listwise deletion by STATA was appropriate

to handle missing information. Missing information within a data frame may compromise

inferences from obtained results. In the present study, results of Little’s test suggest that

variable of interests were not (p < .05) missing completely at random (MCAR), with a range of

0.5 percent to 15 percent. Therefore, multiple imputation by chained equations (MICE) was

employed for all variables included in the analytic models because MICE does not assume a joint

multivariate normal distribution for categorical variables [40]. Ten cycles of imputation were

carried out.

Page 9 of 18

181

Zheng, H., Elliott, W., & O’Brien, M. (2022). The Importance of Parent/Child Communications About Children’s Savings Accounts for Developing a

College-Bound Identity. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 9(9). 173-190.

URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.99.13030

The second step addressed the potential for selection bias that could arise when only

respondents who were Promise Scholars were included. To address potential selection bias, an

Inverse Probability Weight (IPW) for survey participants who were Promise Scholars and those

who were not was constructed. Briefly, we used information available for all survey

respondents to predict the probability of signing up for Promise Scholars and used the inverse

of the probabilities as weights in the analysis so that the results would be representative for the

initial populations of the sample [41]. Inverse probability weights were estimated with a

multinomial logistic regression model that used Promise Scholars account ownership as the

dependent variable and all available covariates as independent variables [42].

All covariates described above were included in the weight estimation, as they were thought to

be potentially correlated with signing up for Promise Scholars. Balance checks were conducted

to determine the ability of IPW to balance relevant covariates. Multinomial logistic models that

used Promise Scholar account ownership as the dependent variable and each covariate as the

independent variable were estimated. P-values of the coefficients from each model before and

after applying inverse probability weights are displayed. In the unweighted sample, many of

the covariates showed significant group differences between respondents who signed up for

Promise Scholars and those who did not. Once the inverse probability weight was applied,

group differences on all covariates were no longer significant (see Table 1).

Table 1. Data Balance Before & After Propensity Score Inverse-Probability Weight Adjustmenta

Promise Scholars vs.

Comparison

Promise Indiana

Participants vs.

Comparison Covariates Unweighted

P-Value

Weighted

P-Value

Unweighted

P-Value

Weighted

P-Value

Household net worth: Negative .124 .374 .847 .982

Household net worth: $0-$55,000 .050 .929 .708 .289

Household net worth: $55,001-$150,000 .696 .206 .9.3 .724

Household net worth: > $150,001 .002 .454 .324 .211

Household income: < $55,000 <.001 .973 .608 .905

Household income: $55,001-$95,000 .552 .922 .452 .198

Household income: > $95,001 <.001 .953 .058 .259

Marital status <0.001 0.976 0.176 0.551

Child gender 0.769 0.883 0.707 0.971

Child’s grade: 5th grade .477 .560 .198 .821

Child’s grade: 6th grade .971 .426 .334 .316

Child’s grade: 7th grade .510 .917 .917 .660

Child’s grade: 9th grade .900 .776 .520 .800

Child’s health status 0.115 0.967 0.695 0.413

Overall academic performance <0.001 0.618 0.451 0.936

a P-values of the coefficient of Promise Scholars and Promise Indiana participants are

displayed

The third step was to analyze children’s college-bound identity using ordinary least squares

regression (OLS). Data analysis steps were conducted using STATA [43]. Ordinary least squares

regression was used to predict scores of children’s college bound identity. The mi estimate: reg

commands in STATA were used to perform OLS regression across the ten imputed data sets.

Page 10 of 18

182

Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ) Vol. 9, Issue 9, September-2022

Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom

Joint tests of transformed coefficients were conducted after multiple imputations estimates

were obtained for selected groups [44]. Findings at a significance level of p < .05 were noted in

the tables.

RESULTS

Descriptive results

Table 2 displayed descriptive statistics by communication status. There were more high-net

worth and high-income households in the communicator group than in the non-communicator

group and the comparison group. More households in the communicator group were married

than in the other two groups. More children in the communicator group were rated excellent

health and above average academic performance by their parents than the other two groups.

Regarding the variables of interest, children in the communicator group had the highest

college-bound identity mean scores (19.69, standard deviation 2.93), while those in the

comparison group had the lowest mean scores (17.58, standard deviation 4.17).

Page 11 of 18

183

Zheng, H., Elliott, W., & O’Brien, M. (2022). The Importance of Parent/Child Communications About Children’s Savings Accounts for Developing a

College-Bound Identity. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 9(9). 173-190.

URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.99.13030

Table 2. Descriptive Statistics by the Communication Statusa (N = 506)

Communicator

(%)

Non-Communicator

(%)

Comparison

Group(%)

Categorical Variables

Net worth

Negative net worth 20 (4) 23 (5) 16 (3)

0 - $55,000 80 (16) 81 (16) 50 (10)

$55,001 - $150,000 66 (13) 36 (7) 23 (5)

$150,001 or more 72 (14) 24 (5) 9 (2)

Income

Less than $55,000 75 (14) 86 (16) 61 (12)

$55,001-$95,000 102 (19) 57 (11) 36 (7)

$95,001 or above 70 (13) 35 (7) 8 (2)

Marital status

Not married 45 (8) 60 (11) 45 (8)

Married 213 (39) 125 (23) 64 (12)

Child Gender

Male 130 (22) 100 (17) 55 (10)

Female 131 (23) 105 (18) 60 (10)

Child Grade

5th grade 72 (13) 54 (10) 31 (6)

6th grade 73 (13) 50 (9) 27 (5)

7th grade 62 (11) 58 (11) 23 (4)

9th grade 48 (9) 37 (7) 18 (3)

Child Health

Very good or worse 103 (18) 79 (14) 55 (10)

Excellent 159 (28) 113 (20) 59 (10)

Academic Performance

Average or below 68 (12) 69 (12) 66 (12)

Above average 194 (35) 120 (21) 45 (8)

Variables of Interest

Educational expectations

Less than four-year college 71 (12) 72 (13) 65 (11)

Four-year college or above 190 (33) 126 (22) 49 (9)

Communicator Non-Communicator Comparison Group

Continuous Variable Mean (S.D.) Mean (S.D.) Mean (S.D.)

College-Bound Identity 19.69 (2.93) 18.48 (3.79) 17.58 (4.17)

Note. Missing information are not included for the calculation of descriptive statistics

OLS regression results

We examined the effect of communication about Promise Scholars on students’ college-bound

identity using weighted OLS regression. Students in the communicators group had stronger

college-bound identity than their counterparts in the non-communicators group and the

comparison group. On average, students in the communicators group had 1.24 points higher

college-bound identity scores than those in the comparison group, holding other variables

constant (β = 1.24, p < .05). Students in the non-communicators group did not have statistically

Page 12 of 18

184

Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ) Vol. 9, Issue 9, September-2022

Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom

different college-bound identity scores than children in the comparison group (p > .05). Among

covariates, the college-bound identity scores for students whose parents perceived them

having above average academic performance were on average 1.20 points higher than that of

students whose parents perceived them having average or below academic performance (β =

1.20, p < .05). On average, sixth graders had 1.58 points higher college-bound identity scores

than fifth graders (β = 1.58, p < .05), while the college-bound identity of students in other grades

were not significantly from fifth graders.

Parent/child Communication

Given the insignificant coefficient of non-communicators on college-bound identity, the

difference between communicators and non-communicators might not be statistically

significant. Therefore, this study examined the difference between communicators and non- communicators in terms of their coefficients. The results of the difference test indicated that

children in the communicators group had stronger college-bound identity than students in the

non-communicators group. On average, the college-bound identity scores for the

communicators group were 0.81 points higher than for the non-communicators group (F1, 391 =

4.21, p < .05).

Page 13 of 18

185

Zheng, H., Elliott, W., & O’Brien, M. (2022). The Importance of Parent/Child Communications About Children’s Savings Accounts for Developing a

College-Bound Identity. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 9(9). 173-190.

URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.99.13030

Table 3. The Estimated OLS Model of Children’s College-Bound Identity (N = 506)

Coef. Std. Err. [95% Conf. Interval]

Constant 16.08*** 0.81 14.48 17.68*

Net worth

Negative net worth - - - -

0 - $55,000 -0.30 0.57 -1.42 0.83

$55,001 - $150,000 -0.81 0.72 -2.23 0.61

$150,001 or more 0.47 0.76 -1.03 1.96

Income

Less than $55,000 - - - -

$55,001-$95,000 0.50 0.69 -0.86 1.85

$95,001 or above -0.01 0.86 -1.71 1.69

Marital status

Not married - - - -

Married -0.15 0.62 -1.38 1.07

Child Gender

Male - - - -

Female 0.21 0.49 -0.75 1.16

Child Grade

5th grade - - - -

6th grade 1.58* 0.56 0.47 2.69

7th grade 0.74 0.80 -0.83 2.31

9th grade 0.36 0.70 -1.02 1.75

Child Health

Very good or worse - - - -

Excellent 0.90 0.54 -0.17 1.96

Academic Performance

Average or below - - - -

Above average 1.20* 0.56 0.11 2.30

Educational expectations

Less than four-year college - - - -

Four-year college or above 0.40 0.68 -0.95 1.74

Communication Status

Comparison group - - - -

Non-Communicator 0.43 0.53 -0.62 1.47

Communicator 1.24* 0.50 0.24 2.23

* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001, two-tailed test

DISCUSSION

This paper investigated the relationship between parental communications about CSA

programs with their children and children’s formation of a college-bound identity. This line of

investigation came out of early thinking on CSAs that suggests in order to produce the types of

social and psychological effects Sherraden [3] originally hypothesized, children must know that

they have an account. This question about parental communications has practical implications

for the CSA field. If parental communications are associated with improved outcomes with CSA

programs, then developing interventions that help foster this communication may be important

Page 14 of 18

186

Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ) Vol. 9, Issue 9, September-2022

Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom

for strengthening CSA effects. Moreover, this is the type of intervention that programs can

relatively easily implement without incurring high costs, making it all the more important to

understand.

While no existing research specifically tests the relationship between parental communications

about CSA programs with their children and children’s college-bound identities, there is

evidence to suggest that CSAs can influence parenting practices more generally [21, 19, 45].

Parenting communications fall under the broad research on parenting practices. Findings from

this study indicate that parental communications about the program are associated with

children’s formation of a college-bound identity. This is important because college expectations,

a proxy for college-bound identity, have been shown to be important factors for predicting

children’s education outcomes [10-12].

Furthermore, findings from this study align with findings on the relationship between CSAs and

children’s college expectations. Prior research has found that children who have savings set

aside for college, a proxy for CSAs, is positively associated with children’s college expectations

(e.g., [10, 46-47]). In this study, however, instead of using a single survey question (“do you

expect to attend college”) as a proxy for children’s formation of a college-bound identity, the

researchers used a new measure of college-bound identity which consists of three subscales

with 27 items. The three subscales are meant to capture the three core components of a college- bound identity: salience (college is on the forefront of the mind), difficulty as normal

(schoolwork is supposed to be hard and I have strategies to succeed), and group congruence

(people like me go to college).

Findings from this study also align with studies conducted by identity-based motivation

researchers. Specifically, we find that parent-child communications about CSAs is associated

with children developing a college-bound identity. This aligns with research conducted by IBM

researchers that finds parent-child communications are associated with children forming

possible identities related to career and schooling [33-35, 28]. Findings from this study are also

consistent with IBM research that indicates that possible identities are co-constructed [35].

That is, both parent and child can play a role in the formation of a child’s college-bound identity.

Implications

The primary implication drawn from this study is that CSA programs should consider

implementing strategies that encourage families to talk to their children about their CSA if a

goal of the program is developing a college-bound identity among their participants. There are

many ways that programs may encourage families to communicate with their children about

their CSA and college. For example, programs could send out text messages to families

reminding them of the importance of talking to their children about their CSA. They could send

out more frequent financial statements and encourage parents to sit down and talk to their

children about the statement. Another thing that programs can do is encourage families to make

depositing funds a parent-child event. Certainly, there are additional ways that programs could

encourage parents to talk to their children about their CSA, but these are a few.

Another implication of this study is the idea that there are ways that families can engage with

the CSA program that go beyond making deposits. Often CSA programs too narrowly define

engagement as being whether families make a deposit or simply end up prioritizing making