Page 1 of 9

Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal – Vol. 8, No. 8

Publication Date: August 25, 2021

DOI:10.14738/assrj.88.10776. Uji, M., & Kawaguchi, M. (2021). Psychometric Properties of Object Relation Scale: Factor Structure and Relationship to Mental

Health. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 8(8). 616-624.

Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom

Psychometric Properties of Object Relation Scale: Factor

Structure and Relationship to Mental Health

Masayo Uji

Department of Nursing, Yasuda Women’s University, Hiroshima, JAPAN

Makiko Kawaguchi

Kyushu Chuo Rehabilitation Gakuin

ABSTRACT

Background: Object Relations Scale (ORS: Iume, Hirai, Aoki & Baba, 2006) was

developed for accessing an individual’s object relation pattern. It consists of five

domains: Insufficiency of Intimacy, Superficiality in Interpersonal Relations,

Egoistic Manipulation, Excessive Need for Identification, and Abandonment Anxiety.

However, its factor structure using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) is still

undetermined. Purpose: This study aims at evaluating the psychometric properties

of ORS, in particular, confirming its factor structure using CFA, and examining the

relationship of object relation maturity, to mental health as well as to psychological

distress. Methods: The subjects of this study were 547 medical college students in

Japan. CFA were conducted in order to determine the best fit model. The

relationships of maturity level of one’s object relation pattern to his/her mental

health as well as psychological distresses were examined by t-tests. Results: A four- factor model, a modified version of the original five-factor model showed the best

fit. Among the four factors, three were those included in the original model. They

were Insufficiency of Intimacy, Superficiality in Interpersonal Relations, and

Abandonment Anxiety. The last factor consisted of items originally included in the

remaining two factors, Egotistic Manipulation and Excessive Need for Identification.

Each ORS subscale score positively correlated with that of mental health problems

as well as those of psychological distresses of one or more domains at significant

levels. Conclusion: The four-factor model, which does not necessarily negate the

original five-factor model proposed by Iume et al., showed the best fit. Immaturity

and instability in object relation relationship contributed to a variety of distresses

as well as poor mental health.

Keywords: Object Relation Scale, Factor structure, Mental health, Psychological

distresses.

INTRODUCTION

The concept of object relations was originally developed in the psychoanalytic realm. Theories

concerning object as well as object relations have been differentiated after Freud who focused

on instinctual drives. Some scholars (Ainsworth, 1969; Bellack, 1973; Fujiyama, 2002;

Greenberg, & Mitchell, 1983) have reviewed theories on object relations proposed by

psychoanalysts from a wide range of schools. The main differences between the schools are the

origin and development processes of object relations. Klein (1946) described object relation as

phenomena within an infant’s instinctual phantasy where projection and introjection

Page 2 of 9

617

Uji, M., & Kawaguchi, M. (2021). Psychometric Properties of Object Relation Scale: Factor Structure and Relationship to Mental Health. Advances

in Social Sciences Research Journal, 8(8). 616-624.

URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.88.10776

incessantly occur. The concept of internal object was the product of re-introjection. She did not

mention much about roles taken by an external object, the actual mother. She identified the

origin of infant’s object in its innate phylogenetic images of human body organs. Based on these

images the infant’s phantasy concerning the dynamic interaction between its ego and the object

is developed in a very graphic manner, more specifically, projection and re-introjection of body

parts or body organ contents. Although the object she described was non-specific, her work

contributed in bridging Freud’s ego psychology and later object relation theories by shifting the

main theme from instinctual drive to object relation.

Jacobson (1964), one of Freud’s successors in the ego psychology school, discussed that along

with ego development, mental representation of object relations is formed between self and

object worlds. Fairbairn (1994), who took into account the role of the external object, wrote

that representation of object relations are developed by an infant internalizing the dynamic

relationship between external object and ego. Based on the previous psychoanalysts’ theories

on object-relations, Fujiyama (2002) defines object relations as “representation of the

relationship between self and object in an individual’s inner world, which determines his/her

attitudes and behaviors within interpersonal relationships.”

Bellak (1973) tried to assess the nature of an individual’s object relations from the following

four aspects: “The degree and kind of relatedness to others, degree of closeness or distance, and

degree of flexibility and choice in maintaining object relations”, “primitivity-maturity of object

relations”, “the extent to which the person perceives and responds to others as independent

entities rather than extensions of himself”, and “the extent to which he can maintain object

constancy (Hartman, 1952, pp. 163).” The concept of object constancy refers to a form of object

relation in which the infant is able to keep stable trust and affection regardless of whether the

object is existent or momentarily absent. This is independent from the infant’s needs. The object

constancy is one of the crucial roles taken by ego (Hartman, 1964).

In cases where the object constancy is not established, i.e., trust towards the object under its

temporary absence is lacking, good object and bad object represented as partial object (Klein,

1946) are not integrated as whole object (Klein, 1946). In this situation, corresponding to the

object split, the self is also split. During infancy, this mechanism functions as defense in order

to protect good object from an infant’s destructive impulses. However, when it continuously

lasts into adulthood mental life, it brings about deteriorating effects on an individual’s mental

life, characterized by unstable interpersonal relationship as a result of excessive idealization

and devaluation, as well as low self-esteem and lack of ability to trust others.

Iume, Hirai, Aoki, & Baba (2006) pointed out that Bell Object Relations and Reality Testing

Inventory (BORRTI) (Bell, 1995; Bell, 2005), which had been the only tool with a self-report

questionnaire style for assessing an individual’s characteristics of object relations, was difficult

to use and time-consuming for scoring due to its complicated factor structure. They therefore

developed Object Relation Scale (ORS) to assess the maturity level of object relations. ORS is

based on the theories of Bellak (1973) and Kernberg (who integrated Klein’s idea on early

object relation into Hartman’s idea regarding ego-development) and consists of 29 items with

a five-factor structure: Insufficiency of Intimacy, Superficiality in Interpersonal Relations,

Egoistic Manipulation, Excessive Need for Identification, and Abandonment Anxiety, proven by

exploratory factor analysis (EFA), with six, five, five, six, and seven items respectively. Examples

Page 3 of 9

618

Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ) Vol. 8, Issue 8, August-2021

Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom

of the factors are: “I am afraid of getting close to other people [non-reverse item]” for

Insufficiency of Intimacy, “I value an interpersonal relationship in which I am able to gain a lot

[reverse item]” for Superficiality in Interpersonal Relations, “I tend to manipulate others in

order to fulfill my needs as I desire [non-reverse item]” for Egoistic Manipulation, “I am seeking

somebody who I can feel is a part of me [non-reverse item]” for Excessive Need for

Identification, and “I sometimes have the fear of being betrayed by someone who is very close

to me [non-reverse item]” for Abandonment Anxiety.

Although Iume et al. (2006) conducted EFA, they did not conduct confirmatory factor analysis

(CFA), i.e., its factor structure has been undetermined. In order for the ORS to be utilized in a

variety of psychological research, its factor structure has to be determined. Therefore, the first

purpose of this study was to determine its factor structure by CFA. In order to do this, we

examined compatibility of the five-factor model to our data. If the fitness level was not

satisfactory, we planned to explore alternative models that would meet the acceptable fitness

levels.

After that, we examined whether the object relations maturity level was related to mental

health, as well as psychological distresses including not only interpersonal domain but also any

other domain including academic domain. It is easily predicted that an individual with a low

maturity level in object relation is more likely to have stresses in interpersonal relationships.

In addition to their hyper-sensitivity of others’ negative feelings towards them, they actually

cause negative life events due to the following personality pathologies. They have excessive

needs to be loved by significant others. However, they lack in confidence of being accepted and

loved by them, and are always afraid of rejection. They cannot communicate straightforwardly,

impelling them to test and manipulate the others by applying self-destructive behaviors. As a

result, the others stay away, or in more miserable consequences actually reject them.

As can be seen, they are almost always preoccupied, worrying about others’ attitudes towards

them. This prevents them from being able to work creatively by themselves and/or enjoying

activities with others. If they are students, they cannot enjoy academic performance. For them,

studying is no more than stress. According to self-determination theory (Deci, & Ryan, 2000;

Ryan, & Deci, 2000), their motivation for studying is in many cases, external, i.e., to get their

parents to evaluate them higher than their siblings or to avoid the feeling of shame when they

fail. As their ego function is weak, they rarely are able to finish what they started, which invites

other stresses. Furthermore, they cannot cope with difficulties they encounter, due to

intolerance of anxiety caused by ego vulnerability and low self-esteem, worsening the

situations they are in. These lead us to hypothesize that an individual with an immature level

of object relation is more likely to experience a variety of stresses.

To summarize, this study aimed at:

1) Determining the ORS factor structure by CFA, and

2) Examining the hypothesis that an individual with an immature object relation tends to

have stresses in a variety of domains as well as impaired mental health.

Page 4 of 9

619

Uji, M., & Kawaguchi, M. (2021). Psychometric Properties of Object Relation Scale: Factor Structure and Relationship to Mental Health. Advances

in Social Sciences Research Journal, 8(8). 616-624.

URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.88.10776

METHODS

Procedures

This research is a part of the longitudinal follow-up study to explore psycho-social factors

which have influence on Japanese college student academic performance. The study protocol

was approved by the institutional review board. A questionnaire survey was conducted twice

a year, in July and December, starting in 2014. Anonymity and voluntary participation were

guaranteed. Scales and questions used for this particular study were included in the

questionnaire distributed to second year college students in December from 2014 to 2016.

Participants

Among the 618 students who participated in the second year questionnaire conducted in

December, 547 (male: 242, female: 305) answered every item in the ORS. The mean age and SD

were 21.8 and 5.0 respectively. Missing data were analyzed, proving missing completely at

random (MCAR). Therefore, we used the data obtained by the 547 students’ responses.

Scales

ORS (Iume et al., 2006)

We got approval to use ORS in our study from Iume who developed it. ORS consists of 29 items

with seven point scale. Each participant was instructed to choose the number of the answer

that best applied to him/her, 6 being the most and 0 being the least applicable. As mentioned

earlier, it consists of five factors: Insufficiency of Intimacy, Superficiality in Interpersonal

Relations, Egoistic Manipulation, Excessive Need for Identification, and Abandonment Anxiety,

and only the five items included in Superficiality in Interpersonal Relations are reverse items,

and the other 24 items were non-reverse items. The higher the score, the lower the

respondent’s object relation maturity.

Clinical Outcomes in Routine Evaluation-Outcome Measure (CORE-OM, Evans et al., 2000,

2002)

In order to evaluate the participants’ mental health, CORE-OM was applied. The CORE-OM was

developed as a standardized brief outcome measure for use in both routine clinical training and

psychotherapy research. The reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the CORE-OM

was confirmed (Uji, Sakamoto, Adachi, & Kitamura, 2012). It consisted of 34 items, including

dysphoric mood, life functioning, psychological well-being, and, risk to self and others. Each

participant was instructed to choose the number of the answer that best applied to him/her, 5

being the most and 1 being the least applicable. The higher score indicates more seriously

impaired mental health.

Psychological distresses

Psychological distresses were evaluated in the three domains: academic, interpersonal, and the

other domain. Distresses in the other domains consisted of all those not categorized under the

academic and interpersonal domains. The participants were instructed to give distresses they

experienced scores from 0 (indicating absence of distress) to 100 (indicating extremely high

distress).

Statistical analyses

CFA was conducted for ORS to evaluate whether the five-factor model proposed by Iume et al.

(2006) showed either desirable or acceptable levels of fitness. If we found cases where the

Page 5 of 9

620

Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ) Vol. 8, Issue 8, August-2021

Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom

fitness was either undesirable or unacceptable, we planned to explore other models which fit

to the data better. The compatibility of the five-factor model with the data was evaluated by the

Comparative Fit Index (CFI), the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), and

χ2/df. According to conventional criteria, a good fit would be indicated by CFI >.97, RMSEA <.05,

and χ2/df <2, and an acceptable fit by CFI >.95, and RMSEA <.08, and χ2/df<3. We also used the

Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), in which lower AIC was judged as being preferable. The

best fit model was then analyzed simultaneously between genders in order to verify the

configural, metric, and scalar invariance. SPSS version 26.0 and Amos version 26.0 were used

for CFA. Furthermore, correlations of the ORS subscale scores with the scores of the three

domains of psychological distress and CORE-OM were examined using the t-test.

RESULTS

CFA

We conducted CFA for the 29 items of ORS to evaluate whether the five-factor model originally

proposed by Iume et al. (2006) showed favorable fitness, but found that the result was poor

(CFI: .881, RMSEA: .063, χ2/df: 3.198). This necessitated us to develop an alternative model.

The model which showed the best fitness level was a four-factor model (CFI: .949, RMSEA, .054,

χ2/df: 2.597), though the number of items was further reduced to 20. Three factors originally

included in Iume et al.’s five-factor model were replicated in the four-factor model. They were

Insufficiency of Intimacy, Superficiality in Interpersonal Relations, and Abandonment Anxiety.

The last factor of the four-factor model was the compound of items included in the two factors

in the original model: Egotistic Manipulation and Excessive Need for Identification. This factor

was named as Egotistic Manipulation & Excessive Need for Identification. The number of items

included in the Insufficiency of Intimacy, Superficiality in Interpersonal Relations, Egotistic

Manipulation & Excessive Need for Identification, and Abandonment Anxiety were five, four,

four, and seven, respectively. The fitness levels of these two models are shown in Table 1.

With regard to the four-factor model, standardized correlations between each factor and other

factors as well as the factor loading of each item are shown in Figure 1, where each factor is

indicated as a latent variable and each item is indicated as an observable variable.

Table 1. Fitness of ORS to hypothesis models

CFI RMSEA χ2/df AIC

Fitness of ORS to the three hypothesis models (n=547)

Five-factor model (29 items)

(Iume et al.)

.881 .063 3.198 1360.5

Four-factor model (20 items) .949 .054 2.597 553.2

Configural, metric, and measurement invariances of the 20-item four-factor model: male (n=242) vs.

female (n=305)

Configural .931 .045 2.110 954.4

Metric .932 .044 2.036 931.7

Scalar .918 .047 2.183 983.4

Page 6 of 9

621

Uji, M., & Kawaguchi, M. (2021). Psychometric Properties of Object Relation Scale: Factor Structure and Relationship to Mental Health. Advances

in Social Sciences Research Journal, 8(8). 616-624.

URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.88.10776

Figure 1. CFA for four-factor model. The item numbers are those of ORS. The numerical values

of covariances and causal coefficients are standardized.

*** p < .001.

Although the five-factor model proposed by Iume et al. did not show an acceptable fitness level,

our four-factor model is a minor-modified version of their five-factor model. The configural,

metric, and scalar invariance between genders were also confirmed by simultaneous analysis

of multi-groups (Table 1).

Internal consistency

Every item finally selected in our four-factor model is shown in Table 2. Cronbach’s alpha, and

correlations between each item score and its subscale score (Table 2) indicated favorable

internal consistency (Table 2).

Table 2. Items in the four-factor model

Item

number

Items item- subtotal

correlation

Cronbach’s

coefficient

alpha

Mean

(SD)

Insufficiency of Intimacy .84 9.8 (6.7)

03 I am afraid of getting close to other people. .73**

09 I tend to keep people away at a distance by

building emotional barriers.

.77**

13 I find it difficult to get close to other people. .82**

20 I often become tense and nervous when I am

near someone.

.77**

28 I have no idea how to meet and talk with other

people.

.77**

Page 7 of 9

622

Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ) Vol. 8, Issue 8, August-2021

Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom

Notes: “Item number” refers to that in the original version developed by Iume et al. R stands for

reverse, as in ORS 06R, ORS 14R, ORS 24R, and ORS26R.

The relationship of object relations maturity with mental health and psychological

distresses

The relationships between each ORS subscale score and CORE-OM score as well as relationships

between each ORS subscale score and psychological distresses are shown in Table 3. All the

Superficiality in Interpersonal Relations .79 7.6 (5.1)

06R I value an interpersonal relationship in which

I am able to gain a lot.

.73**

14R I have somebody who will help me when I am

in trouble.

.78**

24R I have someone who understands the true me. .78**

26R My relationships with friends are relatively

stable.

.74**

Egoistic Manipulation & Excessive Need for Identification .73 6.1 (4.5)

18 I tend to manipulate others in order to fulfill

my needs as I desire.(originally categorized in

Egotistic Manipulation)

.76**

11 I am seeking somebody who I can feel is a part

of me. (originally categorized in Excessive

Need for Identification)

.76**

17 It’s only natural for my mother (or surrogate

mother) to make my wishes come true.

(originally categorized in Excessive Need for

Identification)

.73**

21 I always feel that I need to be with a person

I’m close to, no matter what the activity is.

(originally categorized in Excessive Need for

Identification)

.73**

Abandonment Anxiety .88 17.2

(9.4) 05 I sometimes have the fear of being betrayed by

someone who is very close to me.

.72**

10 I am sensitive to people’s facial expressions

when I come face-to-face with them.

.69**

12 I get seriously hurt when my ideas are negated

by someone close.

.75**

15 I frequently become anxious because I feel I

will be left behind at any time.

.78**

19 I am sensitive to people’s negative attitudes

and behavior towards me, and easily get hurt.

.82**

22 At times I feel afraid that someone precious

(to me) might reject me.

.81**

23 I feel rejected, when a person close to me is

distracted by something other than me, and

that hurts me.

.75**

Page 8 of 9

623

Uji, M., & Kawaguchi, M. (2021). Psychometric Properties of Object Relation Scale: Factor Structure and Relationship to Mental Health. Advances

in Social Sciences Research Journal, 8(8). 616-624.

URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.88.10776

ORS subscale scores have significantly positive correlations with the CORE-OM score and

interpersonal distresses. Furthermore, Insufficiency of Intimacy as well as Abandonment

Anxiety had significant positive correlations with academic distresses and distresses in the

domain other than interpersonal and academic.

Table 3. Correlations of ORS subscale scores with the scores of psychological distresses and

CORE-OM

** p < .01.

DISCUSSION

In this study, the ORS four-factor model was confirmed and its four subscale internal

consistency was also proven. Furthermore, object relation immaturity assessed by the ORS was

proven to be related to poor mental health.

We first would like to discuss why the four-factor model was supported instead of the original

five-factor model. As noted, the four-factor model can be regarded as a modified version of the

original five-factor model. The two factors in the five-factor model, Egotistic Manipulation and

Excessive Need for Identification were merged into one factor in the four-factor model. This

would be due to the fact that these two attitudes are closely related to each other: an individual

with an intense need to identify with others would manipulate the others in any way he/she

desires. On the other hand, the other three factors, Insufficiency of Intimacy, Superficiality in

Interpersonal Relations, and Abandonment Anxiety kept their uniqueness as an independent

factors, although all four factors are inter-correlated as significant levels of co-variances (Figure

1).

As predicted, an immature and unstable object relation was associated with impaired mental

health indicated by high CORE-OM scores as well as stresses in the interpersonal domain.

Furthermore, two subscales, Insufficiency of Intimacy and Abandonment Anxiety, were related

with stresses in the academic domain and stresses in the domain other than interpersonal and

academic. As mentioned in the Introduction, object constancy is one of the crucial roles of the

ego. When an individual has immature and unstable object relations, other functions of the ego,

such as predicting consequences of one’s decisions and actions, and finishing what he/she

started, are also weak. With students, this can manifest as low academic performance, followed

by academic stresses. Regarding all of the stresses in the domain other than interpersonal and

academic, a variety of stresses were included, such as one’s sickness, parental divorce, and

traffic accidents, among others. Some stressors were unavoidable. However, stress levels

depend on an individual’s appraisals of, and coping behaviors towards stressors. An individual

Psychological distresses CORE- OM

Academic

domain

Interpersonal

domain

Other domain

Insufficiency of Intimacy .14** .33** .16** .60**

Superficiality in Interpersonal Relations .04 .19** .07 .43**

Egoistic Manipulation & Excessive Need

for Identification

.07 .16** .08 .09**

Abandonment Anxiety .18** .34** .16** .57**

Page 9 of 9

624

Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ) Vol. 8, Issue 8, August-2021

Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom

with immature object relation is unable to evaluate some negative life events objectively and

cope with them in accordance to the reality principle.

Clinically, it is important to assess object relation maturity levels for those who experience a

variety of stresses. If a patient’s object relation is immature, psychological intervention to focus

on the immaturity is required.

References

Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1969). Object relations, dependency, and attachment: a theoretical review of the infant- mother relationship. Child Development, 40, 969-1025.

Bell, M. D. (1995). Bell Object Relations and Reality Testing Inventory (BORRTI), manual. Western Psychological

Services, Los Angeles.

Bell, M. D. (2005). Bell Object Relations and Reality Testing Inventory (BORRTI), hand-scoring materials. Western

Psychological Services, Los Angeles.

Bellack, L., Hurvich, M., & Gediman, H. (1973). Ego Functions in Schizophrenics, Neurotics, and Normals. Wiley,

New York.

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “What” and “Why” of goal pursuits: human needs and the self-determination

of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11 (4), 227-268.

Evans, C., Connell, J., Barkham, M., Margison, F., McGrath, G., Mellor-Clark, J. & Audin, K. (2002). Towards a

standardized brief outcome measure: psychometric properties and utility of the CORE-OM (Clinical Outcomes in

Routine Evaluation-Outcome Measure). British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 51-60.

Evans, C., Mellor-Clark, J., Margison, F., Barkham, M., Audin, K., Connell, J., & McGrath, G. (2000). CORE: Clinical

Outcomes in Routine Evaluation. Journal of Mental Health, 9 (3), 247-255.

Fairbairn, W. R. D. (1994). Psychoanalytic Studies of the Personality. Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group, New

York.

Fujiyama, N. (2002). Taishokankei. (Object Relation) [In] (Ed.) Okonogi, K. & Kitayama, O. Seishinbunseki-jiten

(Dictionary of Psychoanalysis), pp. 315-316. Iwasaki Academic Publisher, Tokyo. [in Japanese]

Greenberg, J. R., & Mitchell, S. A. (1983). Object Relations in Psychoanalytic Theory. Harvard University Press,

MA, USA.

Hartman, H. (1952). The mutual influences in the development of ego and id. In Essays on Ego Psychology (pp.

155-182), International Universities Press, New York.

Hartman H (1964) Ego psychology and the problem of adaptation 3-21, International universities press, New

York.

Iume Y., Hirai Y., Aoki K., Baba R. (2006). Development of Object Relations Scale for Japanese Young Adults.

Japanese Journal of Personality, 14 (2), 181-193. [in Japanese]

Jacobson, E. (1964). The self and the object world. International Universities Press, New York.

Klein, M. (1946). Notes on some schizoid mechanisms. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 27:99-110.

Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social

development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55 (1), 68-78.

Uji, M., Sakamoto, A., Adachi, K., & Kitamura, T. (2012). Psychometric properties of the Japanese version of the

Clinical Outcomes in Routine Evaluation–Outcome Measure. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 53 (5), 600-608.